Viref Revista de Educación Física

Instituto Universitario de Educación Física y Deporte

ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Examining the effect of mental toughness and decision-making styles on the competition performances of footballer candidates receiving infrastructure training in professional football teams

Osman Pepe, Mehmet Behzat Turan*, Kenan Koç, Barış Karaoğlu

*Corresponding author: Mehmet Behzat Turan, Erciyes University, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Kayseri/Turkey. Email: behzattrn@hotmail.com

Abstract

Objective: the purpose of this study was to examine the effects of mental toughness and decision-making styles on the competition performances of footballer candidates who were receiving infrastructure training in professional football teams. Method: the population of the study consisted of a total of 48 footballer candidates playing in the U19 team of the Kayseri Sports Club. The sample group consisted of 24 volunteers, who were footballer candidates in the Kayserispor U19 team. To collect the information, a socio-demographic information form, a mental toughness scale and the Turkish validity and reliability study, and the Melbourne decision-making scale, adapted into Turkey, were used. The experimental group received mental toughness and decision-making styles training five times a day for four weeks. The daily training consisted of two 40 minutes sessions. For the analysis of the competition played by the athletes of the experimental group, a computer with MacOS and 5K screen support and the E Analysis Soccer Project football match analysis system were used. The obtained data were recorded with the IBM SPSS 20 package program. Independent Sample T-test was used to reveal the difference between the in-group pre-test, mid-test and post-test data of the scales and competition analyzes, and Repeated Measures ANOVA were used to determine the effect of the training provided. The one-way ANOVA test was used for the pre-test, mid-test and post-test comparisons of the results obtained from the competition analyzes, while LSD test was used to determine which tests showed significant differences. Results: as a result, it was concluded that the 4-week mental toughness and decision-making style training affected the performance of the footballer candidates, who were receiving infrastructure training in professional football teams and positively affected the competition performance of the players.

Keywords: football, competition performance, competition analysis, applied training, mental toughness, decision-making style.

48

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Introduction

As a sports branch, the significance of football is undisputable when compared to other sports branches. It is known that millions of people in the world participate in this sport either as players or spectators regardless of age, gender, language, religion or race. This study is of great importance as it significantly contributed to the psychological and sociological development of players receiving infrastructure training in professional teams in the field of football and to improve their physical and biological performances within competitions.

This study aimed to examine the effects of mental toughness and decision-making styles on the competition performances of footballer candidates who were receiving infrastructure training from professional football teams.

Although people initially experience negative emotions when faced with negative situations, they can often adapt to stressful events and situations that can change their lives. The main factor in being able to adapt to such situations is the phenomenon of mental toughness, which is a continuous process and requires effort, time and individuals to take steps (Garmezy, 1991). Mental toughness, which is an issue that sports psychologists have been focusing on, has great significance for athletes and coaches, especially during competition periods (Crust,2004; Jones et al., 2007). Jones et al. (2007) defined mental toughness as “athletes coping with mental requirements better than their rivals during competitions, training, and in other certain conditions in general, and as a natural or developed psychological strength that ensures athletes are more task-oriented, more self-confidant and controlled under pressure”. On the other hand, in their study conducted on the opinions of Olympic champions, Gould et al. (2002) defined mental toughness with comprehensive content that includes persistence, indomitableness and stubbornness. According to Loehr (2002), mentally strong athletes possess various reactions that enable them to remain emotionally comfortable, calm and strong. Accordingly, there are two skills that are developed: 1) the ability to increase their positive energy flow during a crisis and difficult situations (using energy in a positive way); 2) the ability to think in a certain way to have the right attitudes about problems, pressures, mistakes, and competition.

People who begin to perceive their environment, form their knowledge and opinions in accordance with their perceptions, shape their mental patterns to regulate their future behaviors and their knowledge according to the environmental changes and developments. Human beings are social entities that live within certain groups and since they are not entities that automatically adjust to their environment with instinctive reactions, they perform decision-making at almost every stage of their lives (Jones et al., 2007).

In the most general sense, decision-making is the process of individuals choosing one of many options that may lead to the goal they are trying to achieve. Decision-making has also been defined as the process of adequately reducing doubts and uncertainties while making the appropriate choice among certain options about an incident (Loehr, 2002). Decision-

49

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

making style is the pattern of reaction that is learned and turned into a habit when an individual encounter a decision-making situation (Ün, 2010). Decision-making and decision- making style are as effective in an individual’s sports life as they are in their daily life. In sports, fit and proper decisions have a positive effect, while making the wrong decisions can not only affect the athlete in the game but also affect the outcome of the game. Elite athletes are required to make quick decisions by considering different sources of information (Leveaux, 2010).

This study is of great importance as it significantly contributed to the psychological and sociological development of players receiving infrastructure training in professional teams in the field of football and to improve their physical and biological performances within competitions. This study aimed to examine the effects of mental toughness and decision- making styles on the competition performances of footballer candidates who were receiving infrastructure training from professional football teams.

The main purpose of this study, which was proposed based on theoretical reasons, was to put forward a mental training and decision-making model to overcome the lack of understanding regarding mental training and decision-making in terms of football players in Turkey. The secondary aim of the study was to ensure that the value of psychological education was better understood by footballers and the sports people who are effective and have power over them. Thus, the aim was to reduce various misconceptions and prejudices related to psycho-social fields in sports, especially sports psychology. Practical trainings that show how to overcome the mental barriers that prevent athletes from displaying their full capacity by developing the mental abilities necessary for success in sports and are a part of sports psychology, and sports consultants who apply these trainings can uncover the main purpose of performance improvement and the ways to become successful in increasing stability in performance by focusing on mental strategies.

Material and method

1. Forming voluntary groups

The population of the study consisted of a total of 48 footballer candidates playing in the U19 team of Kayseri Sports Club. The sample group consisted of 24 volunteers, who were selected from the study population using the simple random method (Çıngı, 1994). The reason for selecting Kayserispor was that the applicability levels of the training were high for the team, the footballers within the U19 stay in the kayserispor building and compete among the top league in Turkey's football league.

2. Inclusion criteria

Athletes who have been undertaking infrastructure football training healthily for at least three years without any discrimination based on religion, language and race.

3. Exclusion Criteria

50

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Athletes who have been undertaking training in Kayserispor for less than three years, newly dismissed or high-risk athletes.

4. Experimental Models

Studies with true experimental models have the highest scientific value. Common features of true experimental models are the use of multiple groups and the creation of groups by random method (sampling). Thus, at least one experimental and one control group is included in each study. These are considered to be “equalized” in terms of other control variables. There are a total of three true experimental models. These are as follows:”

Pre-test - post-test control grouped model,

Post-test control grouped model,

Solomon four-group model.

Pre-test - post-test control grouped model: in this model there are two random groups. One is the experimental group and the other is the control group. Pre-experimental and post- experimental measurements are performed on both groups.

The symbolic view of the model is as given below:

G1

R

O1.1

X

O1.2

G2

R

O2.1

 

O2.2

 

 

 

 

 

The existence of pre-tests in the model supports the determination of the similarity degrees of the groups before the experiment and correct the post-test results accordingly.

To compare the average increases the percentage increases in the pre-test - post-test scores can be determined for each group, the pre-test scores can be used as covariate and covariance analysis can be performed with the post-test scores or the pre-test scores (O1.1, O2.1) can be compared and if there is no significant difference, differences between the averages can be tested using only the post-test (O1.2,O2.2) scores (Karasar, 2004).

5. Research Method

As the first step of the study, the personal information form, mental toughness scale and decision-making scale were applied to the athletes in the experimental group. After the application, the competitions the athletes played in on an home and an away matches field were recorded with a camera and the team coach filled in a scoring form for the athletes. As the second half of the first step of the study, the personal information form, mental toughness scale and decision-making scale were applied to the athletes in the control group. In the second step of the study, the participants in the experimental group were given mental toughness and decision-making training five times a day for four weeks, with daily training consisting of two 40 minutes sessions. During the training, home and an away matches field

51

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

of the athletes were recorded with a camera. At the end of the four-week period, the mental toughness and decision-making scales were applied to the athletes one more time and the scoring form was completed by the coach. As the second half of the second step of the study, the mental toughness and decision-making scales were applied to for the control group when the training of the experimental group was completed. As the third and final step of the study after the four-week training period, four more matches on in home and away matches fields were recorded with a camera. At the end of these competitions the mental toughness and decision-making scales were applied to the athletes in the experimental group for the last time and the scoring form was filled by the coach. In the second half of the third step of the study, the mental toughness and decision-making scales were applied to the athletes in the control group at the same time as the experimental group.

6. Demographic information form

The socio-demographic information form, which contained eight questions and was prepared by the researchers and experts, was applied in order to obtain the personal information of the athletes including age, sports age, position, education and familial status (Table 1).

Table 1. Socio-demographic features of the athletes.

 

Variable

N

%

 

 

 

 

Age

18

24

100.0

 

 

 

 

 

4-6

5

20.8

Sports age

 

 

 

7-9

5

20.8

 

 

 

 

 

10-12

14

58.3

 

 

 

 

 

Goalkeeper

2

8.3

 

 

 

 

 

Defense

8

33.3

Position

 

 

 

Midfielder

10

41.7

 

 

 

 

 

 

Striker

4

16.7

 

 

 

 

 

Anatolian High-School

8

33.3

School studied at

 

 

 

Vocational High-School

14

58.3

 

 

 

 

 

Private High-School

2

8.3

 

 

 

 

 

Primary School

4

16.7

Mother’s educational status

 

 

 

Middle School

10

41.7

 

 

 

 

 

High School

9

37.5

 

 

 

 

52

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

 

Variable

N

%

 

 

 

 

 

Associate Degree

1

4.2

 

 

 

 

 

Primary School

2

8.3

 

 

 

 

 

Middle School

8

33.3

Father’s educational status

 

 

 

High School

11

45.8

 

 

 

 

 

 

Associate Degree

3

12.5

 

 

 

 

 

Average

13

54.2

Family income status

 

 

 

High

10

41.7

 

 

 

 

 

Very High

1

4.2

 

 

 

 

 

County

1

4.2

Type of area raised in

 

 

 

Province

7

29.2

 

 

 

 

 

Metropolis

16

66.7

 

 

 

 

Note: N= number of participants, %= percentage.

The applied curriculum

Week

Course

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesday

Thursday

Friday

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Concept of

Types of

Use of mental

Use of mental

Use of mental

 

 

toughness

toughness

toughness

1

1

mental

mental

according to

according to

according to

 

 

toughness

toughness

 

 

the position

the position

the position

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Concept of

Common

Models and

Models and

 

 

Concept of

features of

theories about

theories about

1

2

decision-

decision

decision-

decision-

decision-

 

 

making

 

 

 

making

making

making

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

4c models of

4c models of

4c models of

Mental

Structure of

2

3

mental

mental

mental

toughness in

mental

 

 

toughness

toughness

toughness

sports

toughness

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Decision-

Decision-

Decision-

Decision-

Decision-

2

4

making in

making process

making process

making styles

making styles

 

 

sports

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Structure of

Structure of

Structure of

Structure of

Structure of

3

5

mental

mental

mental

mental

mental

 

 

toughness

toughness

toughness

toughness

toughness

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

53

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Week

Course

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesday

Thursday

Friday

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Importance of

3

6

Decision-

Decision-

Decision-

Decision-

decision-

making styles

making styles

making styles

making styles

making in

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

sports

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Structure of

Structure of

Structure of

Structure of

Studies on

 

 

mental

4

7

mental

mental

mental

mental

toughness

 

 

toughness

toughness

toughness

toughness

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Common

 

 

 

 

Factors that

Factors that

features of

Most efficient

Importance of

 

 

individuals

 

 

get involved

steps used in

decision-

4

8

affect decision-

using decision-

while decision-

the decision-

making styles

 

 

making

 

 

making styles

 

 

making

making process

in sports

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

7. Mental Toughness Scale

The Mental Toughness Scale was developed by Madrigal et al. (2013) and the the Turkish validity and reliability study was conducted to the scale by Erdoğan in (2016). The Mental Toughness Scale is a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from (5) strongly disagree to (9) strongly agree. The lowest score that can be obtained from the scale is 11, while the highest obtainable score is 44. A high score indicates a high level of mental toughness and a low score indicates a low level of mental toughness. The Cronbach Alpha internal consistency coefficient of the MTS is calculated as .87.

8. Melbourne Decision-Making Scale

In this study, the Melbourne Decision-Making Scale-II (MDMS), the Turkish validity and reliability study of which was conducted by Deniz (2004), was applied. The scale is a three- point Likert type scale ranging from 1=Correct to 3=Incorrect. It consists of 22 items and measures decision-making styles. A low score obtained from the scale indicates that the particular sub-dimension is preferred more, while a high score obtained from the scale indicates that the particular sub-dimension is used less. The scale has four sub-dimensions that evaluate different decision-making styles and the internal consistency coefficients of the scale’s sub-dimensions vary between .65 and .80. The internal consistency coefficient for the whole scale was determined as .85 within the scope of this study (Deniz, 2004).

9. The procedure of the study

This study consisted of three stages. As the first stage of the study, the personal information form, mental toughness and decision-making style forms were used. Two matchess, one in home and one in the away matches held as part of the Academy League of Turkish Football

54

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Federation (TFF) were analyzed. As the second stage of the study, the athletes were given mental toughness and decision-making styles training five times a day for four weeks, with the daily training consisting of two 40 minutes sessions. A total of four competitions, two home and two away field held by the Academy League of Turkish Football Federation (TFF), were analyzed during the application of the training.

As the third and final stage of the study, four more competitions, as two in internal and two in the external field, were analyzed after the 4-week training period. The mental toughness and decision-making styles forms were applied for the last time at the end of these competitions.

10. Analyses of the competitions

A computer with MacOS and 5K screen support and E Analysis Soccer Project football match analysis system were used for the instant analyses of the competitions (Espor, 2020).

11. Statistical evaluation

The obtained data were recorded with IBM SPSS 20 package program. Statisticians assisted in the statistical analysis of the obtained data. Independent Sample T-test was used to reveal the difference between the group and also pre-test, mid-test and post-test data of the scales and competition analyzes, and Repeated Measures ANOVA statistical analyses were used to determine the effect of the 4-week training program. The one-way ANOVA test was used for pre-test, mid-test and post-test comparisons of the results obtained from the competition analyses, while the LSD test was used to determine which tests showed significant differences.

13. Ethical aspects

Ethics committee approval of the study was obtained from Erciyes University Social and Humanities Ethics Committee numbered 027 and dated 16/05/2017. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association, and we received approval from a university institutional review board.

55

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Findings

Table 2. Pre-test comparisons of the athletes in the control and experimental group.

 

Group

N

Mean±SD

t

p

 

 

 

 

 

 

Mental Toughness

Control

24

46.88±5.61

 

 

 

 

 

 

-.570

.571

 

Experimental

24

47.75 ±5.01

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Self-Respect

Control

24

10.08±1.61

 

 

 

 

 

 

-.674

.504

 

Experimental

24

10.38 ±1.38

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Cautious

Control

24

9.88±1.90

 

 

 

 

 

 

-.928

.358

 

Experimental

24

10.38 ±1.84

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Avoidant

Control

24

3.00±2.02

 

 

 

 

 

 

-1.151

.256

 

Experimental

24

3.75 ±2.47

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Dilatory

Control

24

3.21±1.93

 

 

 

 

 

 

-.497

.622

 

Experimental

24

3.46 ±1.53

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Panic

Control

24

3.00±1.84

 

 

 

 

 

 

-.482

.632

 

Experimental

24

3.25 ±1.75

 

 

 

Note: N= number of participants; SD= standard deviation; t= independent sample test score; p= significance level.

When Table 2 is examined, it can be seen that there were no significant differences between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious, avoidant, dilatory, and panic decision-making styles of the athletes in the control and experimental groups (p<0.05) (Table 2).

56

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Table 3. Mid-test comparisons of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

 

Group

N

Mean±SD

t

p

 

 

 

 

 

 

Mental Toughness

Control

24

46.42±4.62

 

 

 

 

 

 

-2.004

.049

 

Experimental

24

50.13 ±7.80

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Self-Respect

Control

24

9.92±2.69

 

 

 

 

 

 

-3.801

.000

 

Experimental

24

12.58 ±2.15

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Cautious

Control

24

8.33±1.52

 

 

 

 

 

 

-10.138

.000

 

Experimental

24

14.71 ±2.68

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Avoidant

Control

24

3.17±2.20

 

 

 

 

 

 

.957

.344

 

Experimental

24

2.67 ±1.31

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Dilatory

Control

24

3.79±1.79

 

 

 

 

 

 

2.424

.019

 

Experimental

24

2.79 ±0.93

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Panic

Control

24

2.92±1.69

 

 

 

 

 

 

.841

.405

 

Experimental

24

2.54 ±1.38

 

 

 

Note: N= number of participants; SD= standard deviation; t= independent sample test score; p= significance level.

When Table 3 is examined, it can be observed that while there was a significant difference between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious, and dilatory decision-making style scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups, there were no significant differences between the avoidant and panic decision-making style scores of the athletes (p<0.05) (Table 3).

57

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Table 4. Post-test comparisons of the athletes in the control and experimental group.

 

Group

N

Mean ± SD

t

p

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

46.71±8.54

 

 

 

 

 

 

-3.939

.000

Mental Toughness

Experimental

24

54.25 ±3.88

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

9.96±1.63

 

 

 

 

 

 

-6.762

.000

Self-Respect

Experimental

24

12.79 ±1.25

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

9.88±1.92

 

 

 

 

 

 

-15.357

.000

Cautious

Experimental

24

16.67 ±1.01

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

2.42±1.32

 

 

 

 

 

 

.938

.353

Avoidant

Experimental

24

2.08 ±1.14

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

3.33±2.35

 

 

 

 

 

 

2.389

.021

Dilatory

Experimental

24

2.08 ±1.02

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control

24

3.58±2.55

 

 

 

 

 

 

2.855

.006

Panic

Experimental

24

2.00 ±0.93

 

 

Note: N= number of participants; SD= standard deviation; t= independent sample test score; p= significance level.

When Table 4 is examined, it can be seen that while there were significant differences between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious, dilatory, and panic decision-making style scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups, there were no significant differences between the avoidant decision-making style scores of the athletes (p<0.05) (Table 4).

58

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Mental Toughness

55

 

 

54,25

 

 

50,13

50

47,75

 

 

Experimental

45

46,88

 

Control

46,42

46,71

40

 

 

 

Pre-test

Mid-test

Post-test

Graph 1. Mental toughness scores of the control and experimental groups.

Note: Pre-test =previous test; Mid-test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 5. Comparison of mental toughness pre-test, mid-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Benferroni

 

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F ( 2,46)

= .033, p= .968, np2= .001

-

Mental Toughness

 

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F (2,46)

= 8.206, p= .001, np2= .263

1-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the mental toughness pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control group were examined, it was determine that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores, there were statistically significant differences between the mental toughness pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. Accordingly, 26.3% of this difference was explained by time (Table 5).

59

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Self-Respective

Experimental

Control

Pre-test

 

Mid-test

 

Post-test

 

 

 

 

 

Graph 2. Self-Respective Decision-Making Scores of the Control and Experimental Groups. Note: Pre-test =previous test; Mid-test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 6. Comparison of self-respective decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Benforroni

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F ( 2,46) = .039, p= .962, np2= .002

-

Self-Respect

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F ( 2, 46) = 14.218, p= .000, np2= .382

1-2/1-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the self-respective decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was observed that there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, while there were statistically significant differences between the self-respective decision-making pre-test and mid-test scores and between the pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 38.2% of this difference was due to time (Table 6).

60

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Careful Decision-Making

Experimental

Control

Pre-test

Mid-test

Post-test

 

 

 

Graph 3. Careful decision-making scores of the control and experimental groups.

Note: Pre-test=previous test; Mid-test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 7. Comparison of the careful decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Benforroni

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F (2,46) = 7.171, p= .002, np2= .238

2-1/3-2

Self-Respect

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F ( 2, 46) = 52.238, p= .000, np2= .694

1-2/1-3/2-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the careful decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control group were examined, it was determined that there were significant differences between the mid-test and pre-test scores and between the post-test and mid-test scores. Additionally, 23.8% of this difference was explained by time. When the careful decision-making pre-mid- post-test scores of the athletes in the control group were examined, it was determined that there were significant differences between the pre-test and mid-test and post-test scores and between the mid-test and post-test. It was determined that 69.4% of this difference was due to time (Table 7).

61

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Avoidant Decision-Making

Experimental

Control

Pre-testMid-testPost-test

Graph 4. Avoidant decision-making scores of the control and experimental groups.

Note: Pre-test=previous test; Mid-test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 8. Comparison of the avoidant decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of athletes in the control and experimental group.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Bonferroni

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F (2,46) = 1.024, p= .367, np2= .043

-

Avoidant

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F ( 2, 46) = 5.073, p= .010, np2= .181

1-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the avoidant decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined it was found that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the avoidant decision-making pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. In addition, 18.1% of this difference was explained by time (Table 8).

62

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Dilatory Decision-Making

Experimental

Control

Pre-test

 

Mid-test

Post-test

 

 

 

 

Graph 5. Dilatory decision-making scores of the control and experimental groups.

Note: Pre-test =previous test; Mid-test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 9. Comparison of the dilatory decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Bonferroni

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F (2,46) = .582, p= .563, np2= .025

-

Dilatory

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F ( 2, 46) = 8.245, p= .001, np2= .264

1-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the dilatory decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was determined that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the dilatory decision-making pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 18.1% of this difference was due to time (Table 9).

63

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Panic Decision-Making

Experimental

Control

Pre Test

 

Mid Test

 

PostTest

 

 

 

 

 

Graph 6. Panic decision-making scores of the control and experimental groups.

Note: Pre Test=previous test; Mid Test= middle test; Post-test= posterior test.

Table 10. Comparison of the panic decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups.

Skill

Groups

Repeated Measures

Bonferroni

 

 

 

 

 

Control Group

F (2,46) = .808, p= .452, np2= .034

-

Panic

 

 

 

Experimental Group

F ( 2, 46) = 5.346, p= .008, np2= .189

1-3

 

Note: F= variance; p= significance level; np2= eta squared; Bonferroni= significant difference between two measurements.

When the panic decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was found that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 18.1% of this difference was due to time (Table 10).

64

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Table 11. Comparison of the competition analysis positive moves pre-test, mid-test and post-test results of the athletes.

 

Tests

n

Mean

SD

f

p

LSD

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

129.50

2.12

 

 

 

Short Pass

Mid-test2

4

134.75

4.27

33.258

.000

1-3

Post-test3

4

154.00

4.32

2-3

 

 

 

 

Total

10

141.40

11.60

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

20.50

0.71

 

 

 

Long Pass

Mid-test2

4

22.25

2.87

10.299

.008

1-3

Post-test3

4

29.00

2.58

2-3

 

 

 

 

Total

10

24.60

4.45

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

33.50

2.12

 

 

 

Retrieving the Ball

Mid-test2

4

36.25

5.97

.861

.463

-

Post-test3

4

38.25

2.06

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

36.50

4.14

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

10.50

0.71

 

 

1-2

 

Mid-test2

4

14.25

0.96

 

 

Dribbling

50.762

.000

1-3

Post-test3

4

19.00

1.15

 

 

 

2-3

 

Total

10

15.40

3.53

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

5.00

0.00

 

 

 

Shoot

Mid-test2

4

7.00

1.15

14.400

.003

1-3

Post-test3

4

11.00

1.83

2-3

 

 

 

 

Total

10

8.20

2.82

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

5.00

0.00

 

 

1-2

 

Mid-test2

4

6.50

0.58

 

 

Goal Kick-Off

30.300

.000

1-3

Post-test3

4

8.25

0.50

 

 

 

2-3

 

Total

10

6.90

1.37

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Note: Pre-test =previous test; Mid-test = middle test; SD= standard deviation; f= variance; p= significance level; LSD= significant difference between two measurements.

When the competition analysis positive moves scores of the athletes were examined, it was determined that there was a statistically significant difference between the pre-test and post-test and between the mid-test and post-test scores for the short pass, long pass and shoot parameters. Furthermore, while there were significant differences between the pre- test and mid-test scores, between the pre-test and post-test and between the mid-test and

65

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

post-test scores in dribbling and goal kick-off scores (p<0.05), there were no statistically significant differences in the retrieving the ball parameter (p>0.05) (Table 11).

Table 12. Comparison of the competition analysis negative moves pre-test, mid-test and post-test results of the athletes.

 

Tests

n

Mean

SS

f

p

LSD

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

27.00

2.83

 

 

 

Short Pass

Mid-test2

4

21.75

4.03

6.829

.023

1-3

Post-test3

4

17.75

0.96

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

21.20

4.42

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

21.00

1.41

 

 

1-2

 

Mid-test2

4

17.00

1.15

 

 

Long Pass

38.460

.000

1-3

Post-test3

4

12.75

0.96

 

 

 

2-3

 

Total

10

16.10

3.41

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

38.50

0.71

 

 

 

Losing the Ball

Mid-test2

4

32.50

2.08

14.484

.003

1-3

Post-test3

4

26.00

3.65

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

31.10

5.53

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

7.50

2.12

 

 

 

Dribbling

Mid-test2

4

5.75

0.96

9.906

.009

1-3

Post-test3

4

3.50

0.58

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

5.20

1.87

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

6.50

2.12

 

 

 

Shoot

Mid-test2

4

4.50

1.00

7.747

.017

1-3

Post-test3

4

3.00

0.00

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

4.30

1.64

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Pre-test1

2

3.50

0.71

 

 

 

Goal Kick-Off

Mid-test2

4

2.50

0.58

6.456

.026

1-3

Post-test3

4

1.75

0.50

 

 

 

 

 

Total

10

2.40

0.84

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Note: Pre-test=previous test; Mid-test= middle test; SD= standard deviation; f= variance; p= significance level; LSD= significant difference between two measurements.

66

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

When the competition analysis negative moves scores of athletes were examined, it was determined that there were statistically significant differences between the pre-test and post-test scores for the short pass, losing the ball, dribbling, shoot and goal kick-off parameters, and between the pre-test and mid-test, between the pre-test and post-test, and between the mid-test and post-test scores for the long pass parameter (p<0.05) (Table 12).

Discussion and conclusion

Football, referred to as the game of the age, is an interesting sports branch since it includes the unpredictable and exciting movements of players. It is not exactly known where and by whom it was first played, however today, it is played by both men and women in almost every country in the world.

The fact that football is not just a game but a profession and the results of football matches gain importance in terms of both finance and prestige has caused football matches to create a stress factor for a player. In addition, the characteristics of footballers or many other reasons caused by the management or coaches can affect the performance of the players (Başer, 1994). The concept of performance is defined as the highest level of success that a person can achieve in a sports branch (İnal, 1998). Indisputably, athletic success factors must involve the development of physical, mental and psychological performance.

The factors that contribute to this development are the quality of the game, training environment, mental, physical and technical capacity, which is directly proportional to the performance limits of the athletes. Physical training should include flexibility, correct warm- up and stretching, agility, aerobic and anaerobic endurance, strength, balance and stability. Mental training should include elements such as concentration, responsibility, discipline, self-confidence, personal motivation, setting up a goal position, dealing with winning and losing, mental toughness and understanding of play (Özkan, 2016).

In light of this information obtained in the literature review, when the mental toughness and decision-making style scores are examined:

It can be seen from Table 2 that there were no significant differences between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious, avoidant, dilatory and panic decision-making styles of the athletes in the control and experimental groups (p<0.05).

It can be seen from Table 3 that while there were significant differences between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious and dilatory decision-making style scores of the athletes in the control and experimental groups, there were no significant differences between the avoidant and panic decision-making style scores of the athletes (p<0.05).

It can be seen from Table 4 that while there were significant differences between the mental toughness, self-respect, cautious, dilatory and panic decision-making style scores of the

67

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

athletes in the control and experimental groups, there were no significant differences between the avoidant decision-making style scores of the athletes (p<0.05).

When the mental toughness pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was observed that there were no statistically significant difference between the scores of the athletes in the control group, while there were statistically significant differences between the mental toughness pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 26.3% of this difference was due to by time (Table 5).

When the self-respective decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was observed that there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, while there were statistically significant differences between the self-respective decision-making pre-test and mid-test scores and between the pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 38.2% of this difference was due to time (Table 6).

When the careful decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes in the control group were examined, it was determined that there were significant differences between the mid-test and pre-test scores and between the post-test and mid-test scores. Additionally, 23.8% of this difference was explained by time. When the careful decision-making pre-mid- post-test scores of the athletes in the control group were examined, it was determined that there were significant differences between the pre-test and mid-test and post-test scores and between the mid-test and post-test. It was determined that 69.4% of this difference was due to time (Table 7).

When the avoidant decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined it was found that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the avoidant decision-making pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. In addition, 18.1% of this difference was explained by time (Table 8).

When the dilatory decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was determined that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the dilatory decision-making pre-test and post-test scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 18.1% of this difference was due to time (Table 9).

When the panic decision-making pre-mid-post-test scores of the athletes were examined, it was found that while there were no statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes the control group, there were statistically significant differences between the scores of the athletes in the experimental group. It was determined that 18.1% of this difference was due to time (Table 10).

68

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

When the competition analysis positive moves scores of the athletes were examined, it was determined that there was a statistically significant difference between the pre-test and post-test and between the mid-test and post-test scores for the short pass, long pass and shoot parameters. Furthermore, while there were significant differences between the pre- test and mid-test scores, between the pre-test and post-test and between the mid-test and post-test scores in dribbling and goal kick-off scores (p<0.05), there were no statistically significant differences in the retrieving the ball parameter (p>0.05).

When the competition analysis negative moves scores of athletes were examined, it was determined that there were statistically significant differences between the pre-test and post-test scores for the short pass, losing the ball, dribbling, shoot and goal kick-off parameters, and between the pre-test and mid-test, between the pre-test and post-test, and between the mid-test and post-test scores for the long pass parameter (p<0.05).

In conclusion, it can be said that the aims of this study were reached, as the athletes participated in the training without missing any sessions and their mental toughness and decision-making style skills increased, which in turn increased their successful short pass, successful long pass, retrieving the ball, successful shoots, dribbling and successful kick-off from the goal actions, which affect their performance on the field and the results of the competitions, and their unsuccessful short pass, unsuccessful long pass, losing the ball, unsuccessful shoots, unsuccessful dribbling and unsuccessful kick-off from the goal actions decreased. When the literature is examined, Güleroğlu (2017) reported that mental training or imagery training applied to athletes may enable a significant increase in their mental toughness levels. In a study, in which mental training was performed in order to increase the technical performance of children between the ages of seven and 10, it was reported that after the training, the children applied the technical practices better and more accurately (Li- Wei et al., 1999). Crust and Azadi (2010) determined the relationship between the mental toughness and psychological strategies used during training or competition with 107 female and male athletes and found that while there was a positive relationship between the mental toughness and self-talk, emotion control and the use of relaxation exercises, there was a negative relationship between mental toughness and negative thinking. They stated that this condition originated from the relationship of mental toughness with the structures of positive psychology. In a study conducted with nine children, who were elite tennis players with an average age of 14.1 ± 1.57 years, the effect of a mental training program on pre- competition anxiety, self-confidence and tennis performance was examined. For tennis performance, players were asked to evaluate themselves (how do they feel physically, technical quality, concentration, the interval of the effort, mental behavior and toughness, self-confidence level during the competition, their expectations from themselves) and the total of these elements were scored as tennis performance, which were found to be (34) for pre-test and (40) for post-test (Mamassis & Doganis, 2004).

69

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

A total of 43 children (25 males and 18 females) with an average age of 13.6 ± 2.4 years participated in a study conducted to examine the relationship between the match performance and mental toughness tennis players. A performance index was formed from the scores obtained during the tennis match for each player and its relationship with mental toughness was examined. The results of the study showed that mental toughness was associated with several performance indexes (Cowden, 2016).

On the other hand, Shanteau et al. (1991) prepared a program to improve the decision- making skills of students who were studying to become nurses. Within the scope of their study, a total of 115 3rd grade students of seven different nursing faculties participated in the one-year research project conducted in the Faculty of Nursing of the University of Illinois Medical Center. There were both an experimental and a control group in the study. A special measurement tool was developed in order to measure the decision-making and problem- solving skills of the students. As a result of the study, it was observed that student nurses required the given training and improved themselves in this field with it.

Taal and De Carvalho (1997) conducted a study in order to examine the relationship between adolescents' decision-making and their desire to control the future, and to increase their knowledge regarding decision-making. In accordance with this purpose, a total of 186 volunteers, 91 of which were male and 95 females, between the ages of 11 and 14, were included in the training program. The content of the program included topics such as taking responsibility, the importance of planning and the importance of choices and alternatives. The students in the experimental group were compared with high-school students who did not participate in the program. As a result of the study, it was determined that while the students' knowledge about decision making increased, their levels of controlling their success, understanding their emotions, recognizing the importance of school life and understanding their place in their future careers also increased.

Mann et al. (1989) conducted a study on the development and evaluation of the training programs they prepared in order to help people make conscious and effective decisions. A total of 76 individuals, 34 of which were male and 42 females, participated in the study. The ages of the participants varied between 19 and 65 and their occupations consisted of businessperson, skilled worker, office worker, housewife, unemployed and student. As a result, it was determined that the decision-making training program provided increased confidence in decision-making and the use of decision-making stages without a decrease in adaptation. Şeyhun (2000) examined the effect of a decision-making skills training program on the decision-making skills of final year primary school students. Initially, the Decision Strategies Scale was applied to 430 students and an experimental group and a control group, both with 40 individuals each, were created based on the results of the students. The results of the study determined that the decision-making program, which was applied to the experimental group, increased the logical decision-making skills of the students.

70

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

According to the study conducted by Afacan (2019), a 6-week mental endurance training program given to 30 football referees improved their self-confidence levels. It was also found that the psychological skill training reduced the anxiety levels of the referees.

Fallby (2000) conducted a study with 121 Swedish Olympic athletes who participated in the 2000 Sydney Olympics and revealed that the performances of the athletes, who carried out systematic mental skills training, improved positively with these trainings. It was also determined that the systematic mental skills training positively affected the mood of the athletes and facilitated them to set effective goals.

In their study conducted on tennis, Kalkavan et al. (1988) observed that the athletes who did the practice and mental training work together were more successful in learning and using their sports skills compared to the athletes who did practice alone.

The studies in the literature support the present study. It was observed that the training programs applied to the athletes psychologically supported them, increased their motivation to exhibit their new skills and motorize these behaviors.

It was determined that the applied training programs filled in a great gap in this field, which is theoretically lacking in the literature, as much as they had an impact on the athletes

As a result, it was concluded that the 4-week mental toughness and decision-making style training affected the performance of the footballer candidates who were receiving infrastructure training in professional football teams and positively affected the competition performance of the players.

Suggestions

1.The duration of the training program applied to the candidate footballers who were receiving infrastructure training in professional football teams can be extended and the changes that may occur in the mental toughness and decision-making levels of the athletes can be examined.

2.This study can be conducted on groups with a larger number of footballers who are receive infrastructure training in professional football teams.

3.As this study was conducted on a specific age group, different results could be examined by applying the same training on different age groups.

4.(Within the framework of fair-play) Such training can be given in order to increase the physical, mental and psychological performance of the athletes who play football as a profession.

5.As football is played by both men and women around the world, similar training can be applied to female footballer candidates receiving infrastructure training in Turkey and the changes that may occur as a result of this training can be examined.

6.The training can be applied to individual sports branches and can be compared with athletes in team sports.

71

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

7.Similar training programs can be applied to team sports other than football and the changes that may occur as a result of the training can be examined.

Declaration of conflicting interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

This manuscript is produced from SBA-2018-7597 project supported by Erciyes University Scientific Research Project Unit.

References

Afacan, E. (2019). 6 haftalık psikolojik beceri (mental) antrenmanının futbol hakemlerinin özgüven, kaygı ve mesleki tükenmişlik düzeylerine olan etkisi (Doktora Tezi). Celal Bayar Üniversitesi. https://tez.yok.gov.tr/UlusalTezMerkezi/TezGoster?key=Mir2lXQK1dkmQ9Ige3PZbqGU MavdZHPGZFcVhHVBPzXPIfvWvyTgdasYEvvBS8gI

Başer, E. (1994). Futbolda psikoloji ve başarı. Sporsal Kuram Dizisi 4. Ankara: Bağırgan Yayımevi.

Cowden, R. G. (2016). Competitive performance correlates of mental toughness in tennis preliminary analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 123(1), 341-360. https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512516659902

Crust, L. (2004). A review and conceptual re-examination of mental toughness: implications for future researchers. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(7), 576- 583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.005

Crust, L., & Azadi, K. (2010). Mental toughness and athletes’use of psychological strategies. European Journal of Sport Science, 10(1), 43-51. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461390903049972

Çıngı, H. (1994). Örnekleme kuramı. Ankara: H.Ü. Fen Fakültesi Basımevi.

Deniz, M .E. (2004). Investigation of the relation between decision making self- esteem, decision making style and problem solving skills of university students. Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, 15, 23-35.

Erdoğan, N. (2016). Zihinsel Dayanıklılık Ölçeği (ZDÖ): Türkçe'ye uyarlama, geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. International Journal of Science Culture and Sport, 4(Special Issue 2), 652-664.

Espor (2020, December 2). http://www.espor.com.tr/index_soccer_tr.php

72

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Fallby, J. (2003). Social support and its influence on the Olympic experience İn Sydney 2000, XI European Congress of Sport Psychology, Copenhagen.

Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes associated with poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34(4), 416-430. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764291034004003

Gould, D., Dieffenbach K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological characteristics and their development in Olympic champions. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14(3), 172-

204.https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200290103482

Güleroğlu, F. (2017). Anaerobik egzersiz sonrası oluşan yorgunluğun sporcuların zihinsel dayanıklılık düzeylerine akut etkisi (Yüksek lisans tezi). Sütçü İmam Üniversitesi, Sağlık Bilimleri Enstitüsü.

İnal, A. N. (1998). Beden Eğitimi ve Spor Bilimine Giriş, S.Ü. Konya: Beden Eğitimi ve Spor Yüksekokulu Yayınları, Selçuk Üniversitesi Basımevi.

Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, A. (2007). Framework of mental toughness in the world’s best performers. The Sport Psychologist, 21(2), 243–264. https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.21.2.243

Kalkavan, A., Erşen, E., Suiçmez, H., & Çakmak, Y. (1988). Tenis sporunda zihinsel ve uygulamalı çalışmanın beceri öğrenimi üzerine etkisi. In 1 Spor Kongresi, Atatürk Üniversitesi (ss.154-158). Erzurum.

Karasar, N. (2004). Bilimsel araştırma yöntemleri. Ankara: Nobel Yayınevi.

Leveaux, R. (2010). Facilitating referee’s decision making in sprot via the application of technology. Communications of the IBIMA, 545333. https://doi.org/10.5171/2010.545333

Li-Wei, Z., Qi-Wei, M., Orlick T., & Zitzelsberger L. (1999). The effect of mental-imagery training on performance enhancement with 7-10-year-old children. The Sport Psychologist, 6(3), 230-241. https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.6.3.230

Loehr, J.E. (2002). Athletic excellence: mental toughness training for sport. New York: Plume.

Luthans, F. (2002). Positive organizational behavior: developing and managing psychological strengths. Academy of Management Executive, 6(1), 57-72. https://doi.org/10.5465/ame.2002.6640181

Madrigal, L., Hamill, S., & Gill, D. L. (2013). Mind over matter: the development of the Mental Toughness Scale (MTS). The Sport Psychologist, 27(1), 62-77. https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.27.1.62

73

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4

Mamassis, G., & Doganis, G. (2004). The effects of a mental training program on juniors precompetitive anxiety, self-confidence, and tennis performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16(2), 118-137. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200490437903

Mann, L., Beswick, G., Allouache, P., & Ivey, M. (1989). Decision workshops for the improvement of decision-making skills and confidence. Journal of Counseling and Development, 67, 478-481. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1989.tb02122.x

Özkan, A, (2016). Futbolda mükemmellik ve yaşam boyu katılım (uzun süreli futbolcu gelişim modeli. Kanada, İrlanda ve İngiltere Örneğinin Türkiye’ye adaptasyonu). International Journal of Science Culture and Sport, 4(Special Issue 1), 93-99. https://doi.org/10.14486/IntJSCS537

Shanteau, J., Grier, M., Johnson, J., & Berner, E. (1991). Teaching decision making skills to student nurses. In J. Baron & R. V. Brown (Eds.), Teaching decision making to adolescents (pp.185-206). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

Şeyhun, H. (2000). Karar verme becerileri eğitim programının ilköğretim son sınıf öğrencilerinin karar verme becerilerine etkisi (Yüksek lisans tezi). Gazi Üniversitesi, Ankara.

Taal, M., & De Carvalho, F.S. (1997). Stimulating adolescents decision making. Journal of Adolescence, 20(2), 223-226. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.1996.0080

Tekin, M., Özmutlu, İ., & Erhan, S. E. (2009). Özel yetenek sınavlarına katılan öğrencilerin karar verme ve düşünme stillerinin incelenmesi. Beden Eğitimi ve Spor Bilimleri Dergisi, 11(3), 42-56. https://dergipark.org.tr/en/download/article-file/297036

Ün, E. (2010). Satranç eğitiminin, problem çözme yaklaşımları karar verme ve düşünme stillerine etkisinin incelenmesi (Yüksek Lisans Tezi). Selçuk Üniversitesi, Konya. http://acikerisimarsiv.selcuk.edu.tr:8080/xmlui/bitstream/handle/123456789/6548

Varol, Ş. (1990). Lise son sınıf öğrencilerinin kaygılarını etkileyen etmenler (Yayınlanmamış Yüksek Lisans Tezi). Ondokuz Mayıs Üniversitesi, Sosyal bilimler Enstitüsü, Samsun.

74

VIREF Revista de Educación Física • ISSN 2322-9411Octubre-Diciembre 2020 • Volumen 9 Número 4