A Melodrama of Psychology
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.17533/udea.rp.e350101Keywords:
Melodrama, Psychology, Subject Matter, Behaviorism, Mentalism, DualismAbstract
In this paper, I discuss critically a debate I consider as melodramatic for excessive and scientifically sterile, namely, what is the proper subject matter of psychology. On one side of this debate, there are radical behaviorism and interbehaviorism, which postulate behavior and interbehavior, respectively, as the proper subject matters of psychology, with an obsession for an ontological and epistemological separation from the rest of science, as well as from mentalistic psychology falsely accused of dualistic. On the other side, there is mentalism, with an equally separatist obsession, especially from biology. This melodrama, behavior versus mind, is plagued with theatrical handwaving that in some cases seeks to force psychological research agendas largely motivated by personal interests and tastes. I argue that this melodrama only hinders progress in psychology, and that in such cases threatens intellectual freedom, acquiring normative overtones in the hands of people with economic and political power. My suggestion is moderation in this debate, supplemented by a greater general philosophical culture and respect for academic intellectual freedom.
Downloads
References
Broadbent, D. A. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon.
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J. y Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. Wiley
Burgos, J. E. (2015). Antidualism and antimentalism in radical behaviorism. Behavior and Philosophy, 43, 1–37. https://www.behavior.org/resources/915.pdf
Burgos, J. E. (2016). Mentalism versus dualism: Replies to commentaries. Behavior and Philosophy, 44, 46–79. https://www.behavior.org/resources/997.pdf
Burgos, J. E. (2019). Selection by reinforcement: A critical reappraisal. Behavioural Processes, 161, 149–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.01.019
Burgos, J. E. (2021). The real problem with hypothetical constructs. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 44, 683–704. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-021-00311-0
Burgos, J. E. (en prensa). Training an artificial neural network in a false-belief task with pavlovian contingencies. En J. E. Burgos, F. W. Grasso, O. García-Leal, y R. Akram (Eds.), The mindreading brains. Springer.
Burgos, J. E. y Castiello, S. (en prensa). Pavlovian impulsivity: Connectionist predictions with implications for addiction. En P. Gargiulio y H. L. Mesones-Arroyo (Eds.), Psychiatry and neuroscience update: From translational research to drug addictions and psychoses. Springer.
Burgos, J. E. y Galeazzi, J. M. (2021). Neural network simulations of a possible role of the hippocampus in pavlovian conditioning. En P. A. Gargiulo, y H. L.
Mesones-Arroyo (Eds.), Psychiatry and neuroscience update: From epistemology to clinical psychiatry, Vol. iv (pp. 303–325). Springer.
Burgos, J. E. y Killeen, P. R. (2019). Suing for peace in the war against mentalism. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42, 241–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40614-018-0169-2
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Mouton.
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Back Bay Books.
Eckard, M. L. y Lattal, K. A. (2020). The internal clock: A manifestation of a misguided mechanistic view of causation? Perspectives on Behavior Science, 43, 5–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-018-00189-5
Gibbon, J. y Balsam, P. (1981). Spreading association in time. En C. M. Locurto, H. S. Terrace, y J. Gibbon, (Eds.), Autoshaping and conditioning theory (pp. 219–253). Academic Press.
Kantor, J. R. (1933). A survey of the science of psychology. Principia Press.
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. The University of Chicago Press.
Kulstad, M. (1993). Two interpretations of the pre-established harmony in the philosophy of Leibniz. Synthese, 96, 477–504. https://www.jstor.org/stable/20117824
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational approach. Freeman & Co.
Mendola, J. (2008). Anti-externalism. Oxford University Press.
Miller, G. A. (1951). Language and communication. McGraw-Hill.
Newell, A., Shaw, J. C. y Simon, H. A. (1958). Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review, 65, 151–166. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048495
Place, U. T. (1956). Is consciousness a brain process? British Journal of Psychology, 47, 44–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1956.tb00560.x
Putnam, H. (1960). Minds and machines. En S. Hook (Ed.), Dimensions of mind (pp. 138–164). New York University Press.
Putnam, H. (1967). Psychological predicates. En W. H. Capitan y D. D. Merrill (Eds.), Art, mind, and religion (pp. 37–48). University of Pittsburgh Press.
Putnam, H. (1988). Representation and reality. mit Press.
Rescorla, R. A. y Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. En A. H. Black y W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Robinson, R. (1954). Definition. Clarendon.
Russell, B. (1912). On the notion of cause. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 13, 1–26. http://www.jstor.org/stable/4543833
Sangiacomo, A. (2017). Malebranche’s arguments for occasionalism in their historical context. History of Philosophy Quarterly, 34, 133–154. https://www.jstor.org/stable/44983511
Schlick, M. (1925). General theory of knowledge (Trad. A. E. Blumberg, 1974). Springer.
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell Technical Journal, 27, 379–423.
Shapiro, L. (Ed., Trad., 2007). Princess Elisabeth of Bohemia and René Descartes: The correspondence between princess Elisabeth of Bohemia and René Descartes. University of Chicago Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. The Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1931.9918416
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The Free Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. Alfred A. Knopf.
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7244649
Stout, S. C. y Miller, R. R. (2007). Sometimes-competing retrieval (socr): A formalization of the comparator hypothesis. Psychological Review, 114(3), 759–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.759
Turing, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 398–410. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/LIX.236.433
Vilarroya, O. (2017). Neural representation. a survey-based analysis of the notion. Frontiers in Psychology: Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 8, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01458
Voss, S. (1993). Simplicity and the seat of the soul. En S. Voss (Ed.), Essays on the philosophy and science of René Descartes (pp. 128–141). Oxford University Press.
Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. The People’s Institute.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2022 Revista de Psicología Universidad de Antioquia
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
Texts submitted for evaluation to the Journal of Psychology of the University of Antioquia must have never been published before, nor been accepted for future publication by any other journal.
Upon the Journal’s acceptance for publication of submitted material, the author partially transfers his/her rights on the article, preserving those relative to its non-commercial use and academic circulation as a free-access file.
Except if otherwise decided, this Journal’s content is covered by a Creative Commons licence “Attribution - Non-commercial – share a like” Colombia 4.0. The licence can be checked out here.